Pages

Thursday, July 19, 2012

Synaptic Diseases 2012


The University of Geneva, and more specifically the Luscher lab, hosted a conference last week called Synaptic Diseases. Since I never got to go to a small conference when I worked in synaptic neuroscience, I decided to attend. The presenter list was stacked, including Malinow, Malenka, John Isaac, Nicoll, Sabatini, Kauer, Bredt, Tomita, and many more.*

Never invite pharma guys to speak

Pharmaceutical companies have poached many neuroscientists: Marc Tessier-Lavigne, Mike Ehlers, and attending this conference, Michael Hutton, David Bredt, John Isaac, and Bai Lu. And I should preface the next sentence by saying all of these men are ten times the scientist I am.

These guys gave terrible talks.

I understand life is different "in industry." Your data is now proprietary, not public domain. You need to shield the specifics of your work from competitors. And as the head of a pharma division, you may not know the nitty-gritty details of all the experiments. But, with the exception of Michael Hutton, the rest of their talks included zero data (and Hutton's was more of a review than a talk). One guy (specific names withheld) talked about potential drugs for glutamate receptors, which consisted of showing chemical structures of existing drugs, and 3D reconstructions of receptors. Another guy talked about how BDNF might be used as a therapy, and how they want to investigate it from the genetic (microarrays!) to the brain level (EEG!). One of them even looked ashamed, averting his gaze to the ground, and stumbling over words.

To me, the whole point of a talk is to present your preliminary, unpublished data. The audience gets the thrill of seeing something new, and the presenter can get feedback. Since (I assume), these pharma guys aren't allowed to present preliminary data, I would recommend never inviting them to give a talk.

With that said, here are the highlights from two talks.

Malinow:

Classically, people think that the difference between inducing LTP and LTD induction is calcium levels: above resting calcium, moderately high calcium induces LTD; while even higher calcium induces LTP. The calcium source for both LTP and LTD (at the hippocampal Schaffer Collateral) is the NMDAR; and indeed you can block LTD with the NDMAR antagonist APV.

For whatever reason, Malinow's lab tried to induce LTP while blocking NMDARs with MK-801. MK-801 is a competitive antagonist that binds to NMDARs differently than APV, and blocks NMDAR currents. And when they tried to induce LTD with MK-801, they could. When they tried to induce LTD with a glycine blocker (glycine is an obligatory co-agonist for NMDAR), they could again induce LTD. Their working hypothesis is that glutamate binding to NMDAR is necessary for LTD, but not ion flow through the receptor.

What about calcium? One reason people think calcium is essential for LTD is that if you include high affinity calcium buffers (BAPTA) in your patch pipette, you can block LTD. However, calcium buffers have the side effect of lowering resting calcium. To see whether changes in resting calcium were influencing their results, Malinow's lab compensated for the buffering by including extra calcium in the patch pipette (15mM BAPTA + 1.3mM CaCl2, which they found via calibration of AMPAR currents), and were able to induce LTD. Their interpretation is that some resting calcium is necessary for LTD, but that calcium flow through NMDAR is not.

Since this is a Synaptic DISEASE conference, they had a disease tidbit. Alzheimer's Disease is in part caused by amyloid beta, which comes from amyloid precursor protein (APP). APP can induce LTD when applied in slices, and this LTD can be "rescued" by APV. However, they found that this LTD cannot be rescued by MK-801 or their glycine blocker. In the elevator after the talk, one person claimed that Malinow has been giving versions of this talk for two years.

Nicoll:

AMPAR function is enhanced by auxiliary subunits, including TARPs, and cornichons (CNIH). In drosophila, it's know that CNIH is expressed mainly in the Golgi, and probably plays a role in ER exit.

To investigate CNIH function in mice, Nicoll's lab employed an approach similar to their recent Lu paper: they floxed CNIH, and then knocked it (the fuck) out via sparse viral infection of Cre. CNIH-2 KO neurons lost 50% of their surface AMPAR expression, both synaptic and extra-synaptic; CNIH-3 KOs were unaffected; and CNIH-2/3 2KOs lost 80% of surface AMPAR.

To investigate which AMPAR the CNIH were interacting with, they applied CNIH shRNA in GluA1-KO mice, and found that there was no effect of the shRNA. However, in GluA2 KO mice, CNIH did reduce surface AMPAR. Furthermore, Western blot pulldowns showed that CNIH binds to GluA1, but not GluA2. Thus it appears the CNIH selectively bind to GluA1.

To check the CNIH-GluA1 interaction a third way, they expressed GluA1/2, CNIH, and TARPs (y-8) in HEK cells, and measured the desensitization kinetics. GluA1/2 heteromers have longer taus than GluA2/3 (4ms vs 2ms). And here my notes fail me, but the gist of it is that for GluA1, both TARPs and CNIH can bind without interfering with each other. However, for GluA2, TARP binding is dominant, and prevents CNIH binding. Finally, they did a glycosylation assay that shows CNIH-KO mice have immature AMPAR.

Their working hypothesis is that CNIH is involved in ER exit, as it is in the fly, and that CNIH binds to the GluA1 of GluA1/2 heteromers. During the question time, both Malenka and Tomita asked pointed questions about possible compensation: for the CNIH-2 shRNA experiments, by the other CNIH; and for the TARP experiments, by other TARPs.

There were a lot of other good talks, but a lot of them weren't about canonical AMPAR/NMDAR/LTP/LTD, and I can't do them justice here. With no more conferences on the horizon, it's time to get back to the paper trail, both mine and others.

* Comparing this conference to ISOT, it feels like the synaptic physiology field is a lot less gender equal than olfaction. In Geneva, there were 6-7 women out of ~35 presenters. In comparison, at ISOT, there were major talks by Buck, Vosshall, Kristin Baldwin, Kristin Scott, and Bargmann, and those are just the people I can name off the top of my head. Not great, but better. Of course, that's nothing compared to the underrepresentation of black people (2/600 at ISOT and 1 at Synaptic Diseases).

Monday, July 9, 2012

Compendium of Analyses, Part III: Advanced Single Cell

I wrote the first two compendiums of analyses last year covering single cell and population analysis. Since then, I've been keeping track of the more esoteric analyses I've encountered, and also found a short perspective from Mitra that covers many spike train analyses. Today, I'll cover three analysis techniques: sparseness, ROC analysis, and entropy.

Sparseness:

The simplest, first. I originally found the sparseness calculation in Poo and Isaacson, 2009, where they calculated the sparseness of odor responses in piriform cortex. Sparseness simply measures how much of a stimulus space a neuron responds to, between 0 and 1 (1 being highly sparse). The equation is quite simple:

Sparseness equation.Where ri is the firing rate of the neuron to stimuli i, from the set of stimuli n. From Rolls and Tovee, 1995.
If a neuron responds to single stimuli in a set, and does not fire for the rest, the numerator and denominator are equal, and the sparseness is 1. If, however, a neuron fires at 1Hz to each of 10 stimuli, the sum of the numerator would be 1/100 * 10 = 0.1; and the denominator would be 1/10 * 10 = 1; and the sparseness would be 0.1. Poo and Isaacson found that the sparseness of piriform cortex neurons was 0.88, indicating most cells there responded selectively to a single odor. You can also look at population sparseness, using the response of a cell to a specific stimuli, in a population of cells n.

A more interesting example comes from vision, from Vinje and Gallant. They recorded from visual cortex while presenting movies; the movies varied in size, with the smallest only stimulating the classical receptive field (CRF), while the largest movies covered an area 4x the CRF. For the small movies, the firing rate was moderate, and increased following some frames (below, left). For the large area movies, however, the firing rate was near zero except for specific frames.

Stimulation of larger areas increases sparseness of V1 neurons. Left. Recordings from macaque V1 during playback of movie in the CRF or an area 4xCRF. The firing rate was higher during the CRF stimulation, and more sparse for the 4xCRF stimulation. Right. Sparseness calculation for populations of neurons for stimulation of different size CRFs. As the stimulus area increased, the sparseness also increased.
From Vinje and Gallant, 2000.
To look at this for the population, they calculated the sparseness of the response, where each stimuli was an individual frame. For stimulation of only the CRF, the average sparseness was 41%; stimulation of 4xCRF yielded a sparseness of 62%.

Sparseness seems like a quick and dirty measure, but of limited application to olfaction or taste. First, chemosensory stimulus spaces are generally small enough that you can simply quantify the percent of stimuli a neuron responds to. Second, olfactory responses are highly phasic (or tonic-phasic as one guy at my poster at ISOT insisted), which makes measuring firing rate less useful. To adapt it to olfaction, you would probably need to substitute a different metric for responsiveness, such as "difference in spikes from control breath."

Receiver Operator Characteristic (ROC) analysis:

ROC analysis is used to tell to what degree, and under what conditions, two responses are discriminable. I first came across this in Cury and Uchida, and they have a nice supplemental figure that shows how it's useful in OB coding.

They recorded from the OB of rats while presenting various odors, and wanted to know: 1. whether a neuron fired differently during odor presentation than pre-odor breaths; 2. when the firing was most different; and 3. what time window allowed the best discrimination. To do this, they compared the firing rates between control an odor breaths during defined windows during the breath (red and blue areas in panels A/B below). For each trial, they counted the number of spikes in the epoch (panel C), and then plotted the distributions of spike counts for each trial (panel E).

Copied from Cury and Uchida:
"(A) PETH of an example M/T cell, aligned by the first odor inhalation onset. Black, odor; gray, blank. The red (1) and blue (2) shaded areas indicate two example analysisepochs. This neuron and these epochs are used in all subsequent panels.
(C) Raster plot of spike trains over multiple trials, aligned by the first odor inhalation onset. Odor trials (above) are separated from blank trials (below) by the horizontal black line.
(D) Response reliability (area under the ROC curve, auROC), calculated for varying epochs (bin size: 5 ms to 160 ms.; bin center: t = 0 to t = 160 ms), as in Figure 2D. auROC values are indicated using the color scale at right, with red and blue signifying increased and decreased spike counts, respectively. The black circles indicate the three example windows plotted in (B). Selection of the optimal epoch was restricted to occur within the 0 to 160 ms response window, the bounds of which are indicated by the diagonal dotted lines.
(E) Distribution of single trial spike counts for both odor(black) and blank (gray) for the same three example epochs.
(F) The corresponding ROC curves for the same three example epochs, comparing the hit rate to the false alarm rate as a discrimination threshold is slid across the distributions. The resulting auROC value is listed to the right."
Then, they asked, if you had to classify a trial as "blank" or "odor" based on the number of spikes in a trial, how successful would various thresholds be? For example, in the top part of panel E, a threshold of 0.5 spikes would correctly classify most odor trials (a "hit"), while mis-classifying a few blank trials as odor ("false alarms"). By contrast, for the bottom part of panel E, no threshold could discriminate blank from odor.

For each threshold, you get a proportion of hits vs false alarms, and can plot this as the ROC curve (panel F). If two populations are easy to discriminate, you will get curves like those shown in blue or red; if the populations are hard to discriminate, you get a curve like that shown in black. You can collapse the ROC curve into a single measure of discriminability by integrating the area under the curve (auROC); values near 0 or 1 show strong discrimination for that epoch.

So that's how you get the auROC for a specific epoch. You can then repeat the procedure for different epoch lengths and onsets (panel D). This panel shows that there are two good epoch to discriminate odor from blank: ~60ms, and ~140ms, and that the optimal epoch size is 60-80ms. In general, they found that excitatory responses could be found throughout the sniff, with epoch sizes of ~40-60ms; in contrast, inhibitory responses tended to be between 80-120ms, and have epoch sizes of ~60-80ms.

ROC analysis is kinda funky, so I recommend playing with this demonstration to get a better feel for ROC curves.

Entropy/ information / bits!

I must admit, I don't completely grok entropy. I (think I) understand the equations, but I don't understand what it's useful for. Calculating how much information is in a neuron's firing without decoding the stimulus seems like calculating the optimal bit rate for an mp3 without listening to the song.

In any case, the entropy of a firing rate can intuitively be understood as the unpredictability of the firing rate, and by inference how much information is in the firing rate. The relationship between unpredictability and information is easy to understand: a neuron that fires at exactly 10Hz in response to all stimuli is conveying no information; in contrast, a neuron that fires at 10Hz to one stimuli, but not to others, contains some information; more subtly, a neuron that fires on average at 1Hz, but with bursts and silent periods, depending on the stimulus, also contains information with each spike.

So how do you calculate the entropy of a spike train? (The best review of this I found was from Bhumbra and Dyball). First, you need to bin the spike train into a histogram where each bin can contain only one spike. This histogram has three features: the number of bins, the bin size, and the number of spikes in the histogram. The likelihood of a given spike train is defined by how often that pattern occurs out of all possible spike trains. If a neuron uses all possible spike trains, the entropy of all spike trains is simply log2(# of spike trains possible). Since calculating the potential number of spike trains involves factorials, and is unwieldy for large numbers of bins, this equation can be approximated as (skipping all the algebra here) log2(e/(M*dt)), where M is the mean firing rate, and dt is the bin size.

Of course, neurons don't use all possible spike trains possible, but only a subset of them. In that case, you simply multiply the probability of a spike train times that train's information:

The entropy of a spike train is the probability of a spike train i, times the log of that probability. From Wikipedia entry for entropy.
"Ok, fine, I can look up the details of how entropy is calculated later. What's it good for?" I searched a bit for papers using entropy, and they seem to fall into three categories: comp neuro papers about how to calculate it; papers that use it for a panel in one figure; and vision papers.

To get a good measurement of entropy, you need to record from a neuron for a long time to get a large set of possible spike trains, preferably including many stimuli and trial repeats. This makes entropy ill suited for olfaction, where the unit of measure is one breath (~320ms), and odor presentation can take ten seconds for odor loading and clearance. Vision, however, does not have these problems: you can play a movie to a neuron, yielding 30Hz of stimuli, and can repeat this hundreds of times.

One of the earliest, most cited entropy papers in vision is Nirenberg et al, 2001. They recorded from many neurons from mouse retina while presenting movies 300 times. Some sets of cells had correlated firing, which they calculated as the correlated spikes that would appear above chance, the excess correlated fraction (ECF). To see whether these correlated spikes were informative, they calculated the entropy of the neuron pairs' spike train both including the correlated spikes, and excluding them (or rather, treating them independently). They found that even for neuron pairs with high ECF, the correlated spikes only contained <10% of the total information.

Retinal ganglia act independently. They calculated the information in a neuron pairs' spike train both including and excluding correlated spikes (y-axis), and compared this to neuron pairs' correlation (ECF). While higher ECF pairs' correlated firing did include some information, most information was independent of correlated firing.
From Nirenberg et al, 2001.